Neurocognitive and behavioral development in young children (1–7 years) with sex chromosome trisomy

in Endocrine Connections
Authors:
Sophie van Rijn Clinical Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg, Leiden, The Netherlands
TRIXY Center of Expertise, Leiden University Treatment and Expertise Centre (LUBEC), Sandifortdreef, Leiden, The Netherlands
Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg, Leiden, The Netherlands

Search for other papers by Sophie van Rijn in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9179-7515
,
Kimberly Kuiper Clinical Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg, Leiden, The Netherlands
TRIXY Center of Expertise, Leiden University Treatment and Expertise Centre (LUBEC), Sandifortdreef, Leiden, The Netherlands
Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg, Leiden, The Netherlands

Search for other papers by Kimberly Kuiper in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6429-5835
,
Nienke Bouw Clinical Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg, Leiden, The Netherlands
TRIXY Center of Expertise, Leiden University Treatment and Expertise Centre (LUBEC), Sandifortdreef, Leiden, The Netherlands
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry/Psychology, Erasmus MC, Sophia Children’s Hospital, Dr. Molewaterplein, Rotterdam, The Netherlands

Search for other papers by Nienke Bouw in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Evelien Urbanus Clinical Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg, Leiden, The Netherlands
TRIXY Center of Expertise, Leiden University Treatment and Expertise Centre (LUBEC), Sandifortdreef, Leiden, The Netherlands
Department of Clinical, Neuro, and Developmental Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Van der Boechorststraat, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Search for other papers by Evelien Urbanus in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
, and
Hanna Swaab Clinical Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg, Leiden, The Netherlands
TRIXY Center of Expertise, Leiden University Treatment and Expertise Centre (LUBEC), Sandifortdreef, Leiden, The Netherlands
Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg, Leiden, The Netherlands

Search for other papers by Hanna Swaab in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close

Correspondence should be addressed to S van Rijn: SRijn@FSW.leidenuniv.nl
Open access

Sign up for journal news

Investigating sex chromosome trisomies (SCTs) may help in understanding neurodevelopmental pathways underlying the risk for neurobehavioral problems and psychopathology. Knowledge about the neurobehavioral phenotype is needed to improve clinical care and early intervention for children with SCT. This is especially relevant considering the increasing number of early diagnosed children with the recent introduction of noninvasive prenatal screening. The TRIXY Early Childhood Study is a longitudinal study designed to identify early neurodevelopmental risks in children with SCT, aged 1–7 years. This review summarizes the results from the TRIXY Early Childhood Study, focusing on early behavioral symptoms in areas of autism spectrum disorder, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, and communication disorders, and underlying neurocognitive mechanisms in domains of language, emotion regulation, executive functioning, and social cognition. Behavioral symptoms were assessed through structured behavior observation and parental questionnaires. Neurocognition was measured using performance tests, eyetracking, and psychophysiological measures of arousal. In total, 209 children aged 1–7 years were included: 107 children with SCT (33 XXX, 50 XXY, and 24 XYY) and 102 age-matched population controls. Study outcomes showed early behavioral symptoms in young children with SCT, and neurocognitive vulnerabilities, already from an early age onward. Neurobehavioral and neurocognitive difficulties tended to become more pronounced with increasing age and were rather robust, independent of specific karyotype, pre/postnatal diagnosis, or ascertainment strategy. A more longitudinal perspective on neurodevelopmental ‘at-risk’ pathways is warranted, also including studies assessing the effectiveness of targeted early interventions. Neurocognitive markers that signal differences in neurodevelopment may prove to be helpful in this. Focusing on early development of language, social cognition, emotion regulation, and executive functioning may help in uncovering early essential mechanisms of (later) neurobehavioral outcome, allowing for more targeted support and early intervention.

Abstract

Investigating sex chromosome trisomies (SCTs) may help in understanding neurodevelopmental pathways underlying the risk for neurobehavioral problems and psychopathology. Knowledge about the neurobehavioral phenotype is needed to improve clinical care and early intervention for children with SCT. This is especially relevant considering the increasing number of early diagnosed children with the recent introduction of noninvasive prenatal screening. The TRIXY Early Childhood Study is a longitudinal study designed to identify early neurodevelopmental risks in children with SCT, aged 1–7 years. This review summarizes the results from the TRIXY Early Childhood Study, focusing on early behavioral symptoms in areas of autism spectrum disorder, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, and communication disorders, and underlying neurocognitive mechanisms in domains of language, emotion regulation, executive functioning, and social cognition. Behavioral symptoms were assessed through structured behavior observation and parental questionnaires. Neurocognition was measured using performance tests, eyetracking, and psychophysiological measures of arousal. In total, 209 children aged 1–7 years were included: 107 children with SCT (33 XXX, 50 XXY, and 24 XYY) and 102 age-matched population controls. Study outcomes showed early behavioral symptoms in young children with SCT, and neurocognitive vulnerabilities, already from an early age onward. Neurobehavioral and neurocognitive difficulties tended to become more pronounced with increasing age and were rather robust, independent of specific karyotype, pre/postnatal diagnosis, or ascertainment strategy. A more longitudinal perspective on neurodevelopmental ‘at-risk’ pathways is warranted, also including studies assessing the effectiveness of targeted early interventions. Neurocognitive markers that signal differences in neurodevelopment may prove to be helpful in this. Focusing on early development of language, social cognition, emotion regulation, and executive functioning may help in uncovering early essential mechanisms of (later) neurobehavioral outcome, allowing for more targeted support and early intervention.

Introduction

About 1 in 650–1000 children are born with a 47,XXY, 47,XXX, or 47,XYY chromosomal pattern, as a result of having an extra X or Y chromosome (1). Knowledge about the neurocognitive and behavioral phenotypes of these sex chromosome trisomies (SCTs) remains rather limited in comparison to other chromosome trisomies such as trisomy 21. This is somewhat surprising considering the disproportionate amount of genes on the X chromosomes that have been linked to brain functioning (2) and the reported congruent effects of X and Y chromosomes on the proportional size of cortical brain systems involved in adaptive functioning (3).

However, research has been fueled by an increasing awareness that studying gene–brain-behavior pathways in genetic conditions such as SCT may significantly contribute to our understanding of mechanisms of developmental risk that underlie neurobehavioral psychopathology. It has been proposed that such a bottom-up ‘behavioral neurogenetics approach’ (4) may provide a powerful tool that can complement the top-down study of populations identified based on behavioral classification of psychopathology. An advantage of studying SCT is that the genetic condition can be identified already very early in life through noninvasive prenatal screening testing (NIPT), which offers the opportunity to prospectively study early neurodevelopmental markers of individual differences in neurodevelopmental outcome.

The number of children prenatally diagnosed with SCT is expected to rise rapidly with increasing availability of the NIPT (5, 6). This calls for more knowledge about the phenotype of SCT to be able improve counseling, psychoeducation, and clinical care through early support or intervention if needed. There is a gap in knowledge specifically in the neurobehavioral and neurocognitive domain, as traditionally the majority of research studies (about 75%) has focused on the somatic/medical phenotype, with only 25% of the studies focusing on the neurobehavioral phenotype (7). In addition to identifying the range and severity of neurobehavioral problems that may be seen in SCT, it is also of great importance to have a better understanding of the early underlying cognitive mechanisms of behavior problems. Similar behavioral problems may arise from different underlying information processing dysfunctions in the brain. Knowledge about the cognitive processes that drive behavioral problems in SCT is essential for identifying the nature of developmental vulnerability as well as the recognition of specific targets for early intervention.

The TRIXY Early Childhood Study is a longitudinal study designed to identify early neurodevelopmental risks in children with SCT. Based on studies in adolescents and adults with SCT showing increased risk for social dysfunctioning, neurobehavioral problems, and psychopathology, one of the aims of this study was to identify early signs and symptoms in young children with SCT. These early signs include behavioral symptoms of neurodevelopmental disorders: attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), and communication disorders. Key to the study was investigating the neurocognitive underpinnings of behavior rather than behavioral symptoms alone. Neurocognitive impairments may serve as sensitive early predictors of behavioral problems in later life, may function as markers for children with an ‘at-risk’ development (e.g. identify those that do not meet age-specific norms for language, cognitive, and motor skills), and may thus provide specific targets for early support and intervention.

Therefore, neurocognitive functioning of children with SCT was extensively studied, with a focus on early development in the age range of 1–7 years. Key domains of interest were language/communication, social cognition, emotion regulation, and executive functioning because (i) these are vulnerable domains identified in studies in adolescents and adults with SCT, (ii) these have been found to be key mechanisms driving neurobehavioral problems in adolescents and adults with SCT yet remain largely understudied in children, and (iii) these neurocognitive functions are developing at this young age showing individual differences in maturation (8, 9).

The TRIXY Early Childhood Study is based at the TRIXY Center of Expertise at Leiden University, the Netherlands, including a range of (inter)national recruitment and testing sites, including the Extraordinary Kids Clinic, Children’s Hospital Colorado, Denver, USA, directed by Dr N. Tartaglia. In total, 107 children with SCT aged 1–7 years were recruited with the help of clinical genetic departments, pediatricians, and national advocacy or support groups. All children with SCT had been diagnosed based on standard karyotyping for chromosomal abnormalities, with ≥80% of the cells showing SCT. The SCT group included children showing variation in the SCT karyotype (XXX, XXY, and XYY), time of diagnosis (prenatal and postnatal), or ascertainment/recruitment bias (i.e. the reason for enrollment in research). This variation allowed us to statistically test if specific SCT subgroups were characterized by different risk profiles. Recruitment strategy (‘ascertainment bias’) included a ‘prospective follow-up’ subgroup (51%), including children with a prenatal diagnosis who were actively followed over time, an ‘information-seeking’ subgroup (30%), including families looking for information about SCT but without specific concerns of their child’s development, and a subgroup of ‘clinically referred cases’ (19%), including children from families with specific developmental concerns. Within the SCT group, 33 girls with XXX, 50 boys with XXY, and 24 boys with XYY were included. In terms of the timing of diagnosis, 67% of the children were prenatally diagnosed vs 33% postnatally. An age-matched non-clinical control group of 102 children (58 girls and 44 boys) was recruited in the Netherlands. For all children, the exclusion criteria were a history of traumatic brain injury, severely impaired hearing or sight, neurological illness, or colorblindness. As part of the longitudinal design of the study, children were assessed during an initial baseline assessment and a follow-up assessment 12 months later, with a subgroup participating in additional neurocognitive training with a post-intervention follow-up.

In this review, we present the findings from the TRIXY Early Childhood Study in terms of (i) the broad behavioral profile of young children with SCT aged 1–7 years as well as specific domains of neurocognitive and behavioral functioning: (ii) language and communication, (iii) social cognition, social adaptive behavior, and autism spectrum symptoms, and (iv) emotion regulation, executive functioning, and symptoms of ADHD.

The broad behavioral profile of young children with SCT

The social-emotional and behavioral profile of children was assessed with the DSM (The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders) scales of the child behavior checklist (10) and the ages-and-stages social-emotional questionnaire (ASQ-SE-2) (11). The CBCL (Childhood Behavior Checklist) DSM scales assess the emotional and behavioral problems that were present in the past 6 months within five different profiles: (i) affective problems (as indication for mood disorders), (ii) anxiety problems, (iii) pervasive developmental problems (as indication of disorders on the autism spectrum), (iv) attention-deficit hyperactivity problems, and (v) oppositional defiant problems. The ASQ-SE assesses social-emotional functioning. When comparing the SCT and the control group across the 1- to 5-year age range, higher incidences of social-emotional functioning problems, affective behavior problems, and pervasive developmental problems became apparent. Risk assessment showed high variability within the SCT group; some children showed no behavioral problems, whereas others showed behavioral problems at a clinical level. Compared to the control group, children with SCT more often had a clinical or ‘at-risk’ score for social-emotional problems (40%), affective problems (11%), anxiety problems (16%), and pervasive developmental problems (38%). Further exploring behavioral outcomes in three age groups revealed age-dependent behavioral profiles. In 1-year-old children with SCT, difficulties with social-emotional functioning could already be present, and elevated scores were persistent across the 1- to 5-year-old age range. Affective and pervasive developmental behaviors were seen in 3-year-olds, and more prominent in 4- to 5-year-olds, as compared to the non-clinical control group. Anxiety, attention deficit, and oppositional defiant behaviors were seen in 4- to 5-year-olds. Moreover, cross-sectional examination of the developmental patterns of affective, pervasive developmental, and oppositional defiant behaviors showed that risk for neurobehavioral problems increased with age in children with SCT as compared to the non-clinical control group. Social-emotional problems, however, appeared to be more stable and persistent across the entire age range (12).

Taken together, children with SCT have an increased risk for a range of neurobehavioral problems already at a young age – a risk that appears to increase and expand across behavioral domains with increasing age. Across the range of behavioral problems, vulnerability in socio-emotional functioning was found to be most prominent, as this showed the highest risk and was found across the full age range.

Language and communication in young children with SCT

Language and communication skills were investigated using neuropsychological assessment (i.e. Bayley scales of infant development (13), clinical evaluation of language fundamentals – preschool edition, including pragmatics profile (14, 15), Peabody picture vocabulary test (16), MacArthur–Bates communicative development inventories (17), eyetracking and arousal (heart rate) measures). These different approaches allowed for a comprehensive overview of various functions within the language and communication domain, including both receptive and expressive structural language functions (i.e. phonology, semantics (including vocabulary), and syntax), social use of language (i.e. pragmatics), and broader communicative functions (i.e. navigating during social interactions).

Regarding structural language functions, results from our studies showed that compared to an age-matched control group, 1-year-old children with SCT produced and understood fewer words and had poorer receptive and expressive semantic skills. Three- to 4-year-old children with SCT in our sample had similar receptive semantic and receptive syntactic language skills compared to children in the control group but poorer expressive semantic skills. Lastly, 5- to 6-year-old children with SCT had poorer receptive semantic, expressive semantic, and receptive syntactic language skills (18). Regarding pragmatic language functions, our results showed that children with SCT between the ages of 3 and 7 years experienced more difficulties with all three investigated aspects of pragmatic language: nonverbal communication, conversational routines, and requesting, giving, and responding to information. These difficulties were not only present in children with structural language problems but also appeared to be a more common characteristic within the SCT group (19). Lastly, when shown videos of communicative interaction, eyetracking measures indicate less orientation to social aspects in 1- to 7-year-old children with SCT, in particular to the eyes of the on-screen communicative partner. Physiological measures indicated that children with SCT did not modulate their arousal levels in reaction to different situational demands (i.e. a change in gaze direction) (Urbanus et al., submitted).

To unravel which language and communication functions can serve as building blocks for behavioral outcomes, relations between initial language and communication outcomes (i.e. structural language and pragmatic language) and behavioral outcomes at 1-year follow-up were examined. Our results stress the relevance of structural and pragmatic language on later behavioral outcomes. Poorer pragmatic and structural language abilities at baseline were predictive of more attention-deficit problems, more pervasive developmental problems, and more social-emotional problems 1 year later. Poorer pragmatic language at baseline was also predictive of more affective problems and more oppositional defiant problems (19).

Taking these results together, language and communication difficulties are present across early developmental stages, and various skills within this domain can be affected. Although difficulties with (expressive) structural language functions have been reported previously, this study shows that children with SCT may experience difficulties with communication that extend language abilities. Both comprehension (i.e. receptive abilities) and production (i.e. expressive abilities) can be affected. Language plays an important role in cognitive and social development (20) and is required to communicate one’s needs, thoughts, and emotions. Language and communication are also needed for learning, reflecting on experiences, and to understand the world around us. As language and communication are intertwined with many other functions, compromised language and communication abilities could have severe consequences for the development of other neurocognitive functions and behavioral outcomes, consequently also affecting one’s ability to participate in society or one’s experienced quality of life. In this group of children, pragmatic language in particular was predictive of a broad range of outcomes; social communicative abilities can serve as an early sign of later behavioral problems and may also help explain the variance in neurobehavioral outcomes in young children with SCT.

Social cognition, social behavior, and autism spectrum symptoms in young children with SCT

Another key area of research was social cognition, social behavior, and autism spectrum symptoms. Our data in 1- to 7-year-old children with SCT reveal vulnerabilities in social adaptive and communicative behavior at a very early age, expressed in difficulties with responding and initiating early social communication and in daily life social-emotional development (21). We also found more social withdrawal during observed social interactions in a structured play situation in children with SCT, aged 1–7 years. Interestingly, we found that social withdrawal is more pronounced when social load in the interaction is high, meaning that social input and demands from the environment are conditional for the formation of social behavior in interaction with the social environment (22).

To explore the extent to which early social vulnerabilities are associated with symptoms that are typical in ASD, we examined the possible impact of SCT on the early appearance of ASD symptoms. The results demonstrated that ASD symptoms, in particular the domains of social interaction and communication, are substantially higher in children with SCT compared to the general population. In our sample, 22% of the children with SCT were at clinical risk for a clinical diagnosis of ASD (23, 24). Joint attention, a pivotal dimension of infant social cognition that serves as an important milestone in typical social development, showed to be predictive of severe social impairments reflected in ASD symptoms in children with SCT at 1 year follow-up (24).

In order to understand how social cognitive development underlie social vulnerabilities in SCT, we investigated the possible impact of SCT on early social cognitive functions and evaluated developmental effects from a cross-sectional perspective. By using eyetracking (see Fig. 1 for the laboratory setup), we found significant differences between children with SCT and typically developing children in basic social cognitive mechanisms of social orienting to faces and eyes and joint attention, indicating that children with SCT are less inclined to visually orient toward social important information and have difficulties with following gaze and point gestures of a social partner. Also, we found significant differences between children with SCT and typically developing children in more complex and specialized social cognitive abilities: young children with SCT showed vulnerabilities in the ability to understand emotions from facial expressions. Similar, substantial difficulties with understanding mental states of others (i.e. theory of mind) were found in young children with SCT (22). These findings suggest that social behavioral difficulties may be anchored in altered perceiving and processing of social information already early in neurodevelopment.

Figure 1
Figure 1

Illustration of the laboratory setup for eyetracking and arousal measurement in the TRIXY Early Childhood Study.

Citation: Endocrine Connections 12, 5; 10.1530/EC-22-0494

To date, there has been no research evaluating the potential effects of early neurocognitive training in SCT. Therefore, we aimed to investigate the efficacy of a computer-based neurocognitive training program in 4- to 8-year-old children with SCT, targeting at improving the understanding of social cues from facial expressions. The study showed a significant effect of early neurocognitive training on emotion recognition abilities in 4- to 8-year-old children with SCT, suggesting that there are opportunities for positively supporting the development of social cognition in children with an extra X or Y chromosome (25).

Taken together, the results presented in this section concerning the impact of SCT on early social cognitive development reveal that already very early in development, SCT is associated with vulnerabilities in social behavioral functioning and underlying early social cognitive mechanisms. We found that SCT impacts social behavioral development from the first years of life, reflected in vulnerabilities in early social communication and social withdrawal during social interactions. Early social cognitive dysfunctions that may underlie social adaptive vulnerabilities in SCT include social orienting, joint attention, and more complex social cognitive abilities such as the understanding of emotions from facial expressions and theory of mind. These results suggest a profile of social (cognitive) vulnerabilities in young children with SCT, calling for close evidence-based early monitoring and targeted support when necessary. Also, our study suggests that there are opportunities for positively supporting the development of social cognition, in the domain of facial emotion recognition, in young children with SCT.

Emotion regulation, executive functioning, and symptoms of ADHD in young children with SCT

Another area of interest was that of the regulation of emotions, behavior, thoughts, attention, and impulses in order to meet goals and adequately respond to the environment (26). Many different interrelated activities are essential to regulation, including having the appropriate emotional response to the situation (not too much or too little), showing the ability to exert (cognitive) control over your emotions and behaviors, as well as showing an adaptive behavioral response to the situation. The TRIXY Early Childhood Study has shown that the development of emotion regulation is vulnerable on multiple domains in young children with SCT.

The first domain is that of emotional reactivity, the ability to register and respond to emotionally evoking events. We found that when 1- to 7-year-old children with SCT were faced with a stress-inducing event (a robot that emits noise and moves toward the child), they were significantly less aroused than their peers (27). This was measured on a physiological level by assessing the heart rate during the event. The results from this study also showed that even when aroused, children with SCT needed a longer period to recover from the event compared to the control group. Interestingly, in another study (28), these children had a similar arousal and recovery response compared to controls when faced with a more cognitive challenge (e.g. blocked-goal paradigm).

The second domain included behavioral responses during times of stress, in other words whether children with SCT showed different self-regulatory behaviors during emotionally evoking events. We found in our studies that used psychophysiological and observational measures that children with SCT have a more limited repertoire of behavioral options than their typically developing peers. For example, when faced with acute stress, children with SCT showed less facial expressions of emotional distress compared to their peers (27). In addition, the amount of facial expressions was less strongly associated with the physiological arousal response, compared to typically developing children. In other words, the concordance between the physiological reaction and the behavioral response was significantly lower. When it comes to organizing behavior to achieve a blocked goal (e.g. toy out of reach), children with SCT had a more limited range of behavior available to them (Kuiper et al., submitted). In order to get what they want, they showed less constructive (problem-solving) strategies compared to their peers. Furthermore, children with SCT showed significantly longer use of ineffective strategies with increasing age in this situation, whereas their age-related peers showed a faster decline in use of strategies that were associated with younger age (e.g. venting and avoidance).

The third domain was the development of cognitive skills that are essential for emotion regulation and the executive functions, including inhibition, working memory, and flexibility. The study by Kuiper et al. (29) revealed that children with SCT are at increased risk for problems with emerging executive functions, from as early as 3 years old, and that those problems appear more pronounced at an older age. Furthermore, impairments in executive functions appear broader than the language domain alone, extending to other areas as well such as flexibility, working memory, and planning. It is noteworthy that executive functioning deficits were increased in the SCT population even when intelligence levels were in the typical ‘average’ range.

Finally, we examined whether the effect of impaired regulation in behavior already existed from a very early age on. By using a sensitive instrument (the SWAN rating scale) that captures the full range of attentional behaviors that reflect symptoms of ADHD in daily life, it was shown that, on average, the level of ADHD symptoms was higher in the SCT population than in the general population sample, in the full 1- to-6-year age range (30). The elevated risk was most prominent in the domain of inattention, indicating significant difficulties with regulating attention. There was also a developmental impact: inattention behaviors associated with ADHD increased with age (from 1 up to 7 years old), more strongly so in the SCT group, although significant differences were observed even in the youngest age-group (1- to 2-year-olds). Levels of ADHD behaviors were largely similar across karyotypes, although boys with an extra Y chromosome showed more and broader impairments than children with an extra X chromosome. In addition to attention difficulties, boys with 47,XYY also exhibited difficulties with hyperactivity and impulsivity.

Discussion

The findings of this review suggest that early signs of neurocognitive impairments and symptoms of psychopathology can be identified in young children with SCT. The impact of the additional sex chromosome on neurocognitive development was found on all domains of interest, including language, communication, social cognition, emotion regulation, and executive functioning, albeit in some domains more pronounced than others and with a differential role of maturation (age). This review also provides the initial evidence for early symptoms of neurodevelopmental disorders, including ADHD, ASD, and communication disorders, across the full range between 1- and 7-year-olds.

Key to the TRIXY Early Childhood Study was to identify early neurocognitive impairments, as they function as markers for children with an ‘at-risk’ development, which may provide specific targets for early support and intervention. It is important to point out that although differences between children with SCT and controls on average were found with medium to large effect sizes in most of the TRIXY studies, only a subgroup of children with SCT had scores in the clinical range. Thus, while some children with SCT may already be recognized as at risk in their development compared to peers of their age, there are also many children with SCT who do not experience any or only mild impairments. Nevertheless, findings from the TRIXY Early Childhood Study showed a ‘growing into deficit’ phenomenon (31): neurocognitive difficulties in children with SCT that were present from as early as 1 year of age were found to become pronounced with increasing age. This could reflect increasing problems that may emerge and present more profoundly with age, in relation to increasing demands from the environment. One explanation for this phenomenon is that neurocognitive functions come ‘online’ at different and later stages of development, due to the maturation process of the brain, making it possible that the effect of early disturbances on the brain may only become noticeable many years later in development. Important to note here is that most of the studies reported in this review examined the role of age cross-sectionally; longitudinal studies are needed to provide further clarity on the developmental trajectories of children with SCT.

Another key result of the TRIXY study was that neurocognitive skills may present as very relevant candidates to serve as specific targets for early support and intervention in children with SCT. Not only did the results show a predictive value of neurocognitive impairments (in the domain of language and communication) with regard to later psychopathology, our results also provide the initial support for the effectiveness of neurocognitive training (in the domain of social cognition). These promising results suggest that there are opportunities for positively supporting the development of social cognition in children with an extra X or Y chromosome.

The study of genetic conditions, such as sex chromosomal trisomies, comes with specific recruitment and study design challenges. Specific to SCT is the low diagnosis rate, whilst the prevalence of the condition is relatively high, making it difficult to include enough participants that cover the full range of potential phenotypic expression and not only those who experience difficulties in daily life that led to diagnosis of the chromosomal aneuploidy in the first place. The approach that was used in the TRIXY Early Childhood study was to record how families came to learn of the study to correct for potential recruitment bias, and three subgroups were identified: (i) ‘active prospective follow-up’ (largest group in the full cohort), (ii) ‘information-seeking parents’, and (iii) ‘clinically referred cases’ (smallest group in the full cohort). The studies reported in this review controlled for recruitment condition in their analysis and found no significant influence of recruitment bias: how parents enrolled in the study did not affect the study results. This may suggest that the children participating in the TRIXY Early Childhood Study reflect a representative subsample of diagnosed children with SCT and that clinical examination of the developmental impact is highly relevant in children with SCT. Having a (prenatal) diagnosis of SCT does not reliably predict what the exact outcome will be for any given individual, highlighting the importance of close monitoring throughout development and to act promptly with guidance and tailor-made early interventions when needed.

When it comes to the specific karyotypes, 47,XXX, 47,XXY, and 47,XYY, most of the studies included in this review found no significant differences between the specific karyotypes on neurocognitive and behavioral outcomes. The overall absence of significant differences suggests that children with 47,XXX, 47,XXY, and 47,XYY karyotypes may show substantial similarities in their neurocognitive and behavioral outcomes, which fits with findings in neuroimaging studies showing convergent effect of the X and Y chromosomes on brain structure (3). Nonetheless, boys with an extra Y chromosome in our sample at some points also showed a slightly more impaired profile. It should be mentioned, however, that the group of boys with 47,XYY was relatively small compared to the other two karyotype groups, and results that included the comparison of children with 47,XYY and the other groups should be interpreted with caution.

Within the TRIXY Early Childhood study, some of the children were treated with testosterone supplements. Whereas early testosterone treatment is considered evidence-based practice when treating a micro-penis in young infants with Klinefelter’s syndrome, far less is known about the impact of early testosterone treatment on neurocognitive development. In order to address this, Aksglaede and colleagues recommend to wait for larger, randomized, and placebo-controlled studies to investigate the potential beneficial side effects of testosterone in children with Klinefelter syndrome. Within the TRIXY Early Childhood Study, we did not analyze outcomes stratified by testosterone treatment, as treatment was solely provided based on individual clinical evaluation of symptoms, relying on varying clinical guidelines, and therefore not suited for scientific analysis. Preliminary results from studies examining the effects of early testosterone treatment in SCT (Davis, 2022, this issue) showed no significant differences on any neurodevelopmental outcomes measured. Future studies are needed to provide evidence-based recommendations to guide clinical care in infants with Klinefelter syndrome.

The TRIXY Early Childhood Study was made possible by the collaboration and extensive effort of multiple international research sites. In close collaboarion with academic medical centers, researchers, and patient organizations involved, over a 100 families with young children with an additional X and Y chromosome enrolled in the study. It is imperative for empirical studies that examine genetic conditions to include a large enough sample to capture the wide heterogeneity in cognitive and behavioral outcomes so often described in these genetic populations. Thus, we strongly encourage more and closer international collaboration in order to advance the overall research and clinical field regarding sex chromosomal aneuploidies. This is also supported by our data, given that almost all of the reported studies in this review did not find significant differences between recruitment sites (US vs the Netherlands).

When it comes to clinical care, the results of the TRIXY Early Childhood Study have important clinical implications. Working in a clinical setting with children with SCT, professionals need to be aware of the variation in (neurocognitive) functioning between children with SCT just as much as the developmental risk for impaired (neurocognitive) functioning. From a young age, difficulties with neurocognitive functions (including language, emotion regulation, executive functioning, and social cognition) can be part of an individual’s cognitive profile, even in the face of average intelligence. For example, children with SCT might fail to meet age- and gender-specific norms on language or cognitive tests or are considered emotionally immature entering (pre)school. Professionals should be aware that children with SCT can have mild impairments on many different domains (e.g. vocabulary delay, poorer motor skills, and frequent temper tantrums) that in typically developing peers might not be signaled as clinically relevant. However, our results show that ‘a wait-and-see approach’ might not be applicable for this group of children, given that many children with SCT had continuous or increased risk for developmental delays. Identifying risk for impairments in (specific areas of) neurocognitive functions can result in specific guidelines on what function needs to be supported during treatment. For example, speech therapy should be considered for children with predominately language delays. In addition, while these results indicate that neurocognitive training might be a valuable component in treating difficulties in children with SCT, it is crucial not to focus narrowly on these specific neurocognitive functions alone but also address the social, emotional, and behavioral development in relation to the social context in which a child with SCT grows up, such as family and school.

The research field of SCT is in need of more longitudinal designs, in order to study the developmental pathways of individuals with SCT from early childhood into school-age, adolescence, and adulthood. In addition, future studies should investigate the role of environmental factors on the variability in outcomes in SCT, including family and parental factors, which may also pinpoint protective factors. In terms of advancing the clinical care for these individuals, examining which interventions are effective in minimizing the developmental impact of SCT would be an important area of future research as well.

The main conclusion to be drawn is that (a subset of) children with an extra X or Y chromosome are vulnerable from a very early age on to numerous important neurodevelopmental domains, including language/communication, social cognition, and emotional and behavioral regulation. Increased symptoms of neurodevelopmental disorders are found in young children with SCT compared to typically developing peers. Our findings from the TRIXY Early Childhood Study are among the first to show that individual differences between children with SCT already exist in early childhood and that these can be predictive of future psychopathology, including behavioral symptoms. It provides support that neurocognitive functions work as underlying building blocks for future development and that these at-risk markers can also be targeted for early interventions. In addition, these collaborative results demonstrate that genetic populations that can be identified early in life (even before birth) can serve as a natural at-risk model to examine early pathways into psychopathology.

Declaration of interest

None of the authors have conflicts of interest.

Funding

This work was supported by a grant from the Dutch Research Council (NWO) (grant number 016.165.397 to SvR).

Author contribution statement

SvR: conception, design, writing of the manuscript. KK: design, writing of the manuscript. NB: writing of the manuscript. EU: writing of the manuscript. HS: conception, design, and writing of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

References

  • 1

    Bojesen A, Juul S, & Gravholt CH. Prenatal and postnatal prevalence of Klinefelter syndrome: a national registry study. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 2003 88 622626. (https://doi.org/10.1210/jc.2002-021491)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 2

    Zechner U, Wilda M, Kehrer-Sawatzki H, Vogel W, Fundele R, & Hameister H. A high density of X-linked genes for general cognitive ability: a run-away process shaping human evolution? Trends in Genetics 2001 17 697701. (https://doi.org/10.1016/s0168-9525(0102446-5)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 3

    Raznahan A, Lee NR, Greenstein D, Wallace GL, Blumenthal JD, Clasen LS, & Giedd JN. Globally divergent but locally convergent X- and Y-chromosome influences on cortical development. Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY: 1991) 2016 26 7079. (https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu174)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 4

    Reiss AL, Eliez S, Schmitt JE, Patwardhan A, & Haberecht M. Brain imaging in neurogenetic conditions: realizing the potential of behavioral neurogenetics research. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 2000 6 186197. (https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-2779(2000)6:3<186::AID-MRDD6>3.0.CO;2-9)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 5

    Loughry L, Pynaker C, White M, Halliday J, & Hui LS. State-wide increase in prenatal diagnosis of Klinefelter syndrome on amniocentesis and chorionic villus sampling: impact of non-invasive prenatal testing for sex chromosome conditions. Prenatal Diagnosis 2023 43 156161. (https://doi.org/10.1002/pd.6103)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 6

    Gadsboll K, Petersen OB, Gatinois V, Strange H, Jacobsson B, Wapner R, Vermeesch JR, Vogel I, Shand A, Nowakowska B, et al.Current use of noninvasive prenatal testing in Europe, Australia and the USA: a graphical presentation. Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica 2020 99 722730. (https://doi.org/10.1111/aogs.13841)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 7

    Pieters JJ, Kooper AJ, van Kessel AG, Braat DD, & Smits AP. Incidental prenatal diagnosis of sex chromosome aneuploidies: health, behavior, and fertility. ISRN Obstetrics and Gynecology 2011 2011 807106. (https://doi.org/10.5402/2011/807106)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 8

    Urbanus E, van Rijn S, & Swaab H. A review of neurocognitive functioning of children with sex chromosome trisomies: identifying targets for early intervention. Clinical Genetics 2020 97 156167. (https://doi.org/10.1111/cge.13586)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 9

    van Rijn S. A review of neurocognitive functioning and risk for psychopathology in sex chromosome trisomy (47,XXY, 47,XXX, 47, XYY). Current Opinion in Psychiatry 2019 32 7984. (https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000471)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 10

    Achenbach TM & & Rescorla LA Manual for the ASEBA Preschool Forms and Profiles. Burlington, VT, USA: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry, 2000.

  • 11

    Squires J, Bricker D & & Twombly E Ages & Stages Questionnaires®: Social-Emotional Second Edition (ASQ®:SE-2): A Parent-Completed Child Monitoring System for Social Emotional Behaviors. Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co., Inc., 2015.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 12

    Urbanus E, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, Cordeiro L, & van Rijn S. The behavioral profile of children aged 1–5 years with sex chromosome trisomy (47,XXX, 47,XXY, 47,XYY). American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part C, Seminars in Medical Genetics 2020 184 444455. (https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.c.31788)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 13

    Bayley N Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, 3 rd ed. San Antonio, TX, USA: Harcourt Assessment, 2006.

  • 14

    Wiig EH, Secord WA & & Semel E Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals—Preschool, 2 nd ed. (CELF Preschool-2). Toronto, Canada: The Psychological Corporation/A Harcourt Assessment Company, 2004.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 15

    Korkman M, Kirk U & & Kemp S The Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment II (NEPSY-II). San Antonio, TX, USA: Harcourt Assessment, 2007.

  • 16

    Dunn LM & & Dunn LM Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Manual, 3rd ed. USA: American Guidance Service, 1997.

  • 17

    Fenson L, Dale PS, Reznick JS, Thal D, Bates E, Hartung JP, Pethick S & & Reilly JS MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User’s Guide and Technical Manual. San Diego, CA, USA: Singular Publishing Group, Inc, 1993.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 18

    Urbanus E, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, Boada R, & van Rijn S. [Formula: See text] A cross-sectional study of early language abilities in children with sex chromosome trisomy (XXY, XXX, XYY) aged 1–6 years. Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence 2022 28 171196. (https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2021.1960959)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 19

    Urbanus E, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, Stumpel C, & van Rijn S. Structural and pragmatic language in young children with sex chromosome trisomy (XXX, XXY, XYY): predictive value for neurobehavioral problems one year later. Clinical Neuropsychologist 2023 14 126. (https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2022.2067078)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 20

    Simms MD. Language disorders in children: classification and clinical syndromes. Pediatric Clinics of North America 2007 54 437467, v. (https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pcl.2007.02.014)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 21

    Bouw N, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, Jansen AC, & van Rijn S. Early impact of X- and Y-chromosome variations (XXX, XXY, XYY) on social communication and social emotional development in 1–2-year-old children. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A 2022 188 19431953. (https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.a.62720)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 22

    Bouw N, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, & van Rijn S. The impact of sex chromosome trisomies (XXX, XXY, XYY) on early social cognition: social orienting, joint attention, and theory of mind. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 2022 37 6377. (https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acab042)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 23

    Bouw N, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, Cordeiro L, & van Rijn S. Early social behavior in young children with sex chromosome trisomies (XXX, XXY, XYY): profiles of observed social interactions and social impairments associated with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2022. (https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-022-05553-8)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 24

    Bouw N, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, Wilson RL, Van der Velde K, & van Rijn S. Early symptoms of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in 1–8 year old children with sex chromosome trisomies (XXX, XXY, XYY), and the predictive value of joint attention. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2022 [epub]. (https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-022-02070-y)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 25

    Bouw N, Swaab H, & van Rijn S. Early preventive intervention for young children with sex chromosome trisomies (XXX, XXY, XYY): supporting social cognitive development using a neurocognitive training program targeting facial emotion understanding. Frontiers in Psychiatry 2022 13 807793. (https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.807793)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 26

    Blair C, & Diamond A. Biological processes in prevention and intervention: the promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing school failure. Development and Psychopathology 2008 20 899911. (https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579408000436)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 27

    Kuiper KC, Swaab H, Tartaglia NR, Cordeiro L, & van Rijn S. Emotional reactivity and expressivity in young children with sex chromosome trisomies: evidence from psychophysiological and observational data. Child Neuropsychology: a Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence 2022 119. (https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2022.2102161)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 28

    Kuiper KC, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, & van Rijn S. (Not) getting what you want: emotion regulation strategies during frustrating task in young children with sex chromosome trisomies. Endocrine Connections [epub].

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 29

    Kuiper KC, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, van Buggenhout G, Wouters C, & van Rijn S. The developmental impact of sex chromosome trisomies on emerging executive functions in young children: evidence from neurocognitive tests and daily life skills. Genes, Brain, and Behavior 2022 21 e12811. (https://doi.org/10.1111/gbb.12811)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 30

    Kuiper K, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, & van Rijn S. Early developmental impact of sex chromosome trisomies on attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder symptomology in young children. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A 2021 185 36643674. (https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.a.62418)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 31

    Rourke BP, Bakker DJ, Fisk JL & & Strang JD Child Neuropsychology. New York, NY, USA: Guilford Press, 1983.

 

  • Collapse
  • Expand
  • Figure 1

    Illustration of the laboratory setup for eyetracking and arousal measurement in the TRIXY Early Childhood Study.

  • 1

    Bojesen A, Juul S, & Gravholt CH. Prenatal and postnatal prevalence of Klinefelter syndrome: a national registry study. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 2003 88 622626. (https://doi.org/10.1210/jc.2002-021491)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 2

    Zechner U, Wilda M, Kehrer-Sawatzki H, Vogel W, Fundele R, & Hameister H. A high density of X-linked genes for general cognitive ability: a run-away process shaping human evolution? Trends in Genetics 2001 17 697701. (https://doi.org/10.1016/s0168-9525(0102446-5)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 3

    Raznahan A, Lee NR, Greenstein D, Wallace GL, Blumenthal JD, Clasen LS, & Giedd JN. Globally divergent but locally convergent X- and Y-chromosome influences on cortical development. Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY: 1991) 2016 26 7079. (https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu174)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 4

    Reiss AL, Eliez S, Schmitt JE, Patwardhan A, & Haberecht M. Brain imaging in neurogenetic conditions: realizing the potential of behavioral neurogenetics research. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 2000 6 186197. (https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-2779(2000)6:3<186::AID-MRDD6>3.0.CO;2-9)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 5

    Loughry L, Pynaker C, White M, Halliday J, & Hui LS. State-wide increase in prenatal diagnosis of Klinefelter syndrome on amniocentesis and chorionic villus sampling: impact of non-invasive prenatal testing for sex chromosome conditions. Prenatal Diagnosis 2023 43 156161. (https://doi.org/10.1002/pd.6103)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 6

    Gadsboll K, Petersen OB, Gatinois V, Strange H, Jacobsson B, Wapner R, Vermeesch JR, Vogel I, Shand A, Nowakowska B, et al.Current use of noninvasive prenatal testing in Europe, Australia and the USA: a graphical presentation. Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica 2020 99 722730. (https://doi.org/10.1111/aogs.13841)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 7

    Pieters JJ, Kooper AJ, van Kessel AG, Braat DD, & Smits AP. Incidental prenatal diagnosis of sex chromosome aneuploidies: health, behavior, and fertility. ISRN Obstetrics and Gynecology 2011 2011 807106. (https://doi.org/10.5402/2011/807106)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 8

    Urbanus E, van Rijn S, & Swaab H. A review of neurocognitive functioning of children with sex chromosome trisomies: identifying targets for early intervention. Clinical Genetics 2020 97 156167. (https://doi.org/10.1111/cge.13586)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 9

    van Rijn S. A review of neurocognitive functioning and risk for psychopathology in sex chromosome trisomy (47,XXY, 47,XXX, 47, XYY). Current Opinion in Psychiatry 2019 32 7984. (https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000471)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 10

    Achenbach TM & & Rescorla LA Manual for the ASEBA Preschool Forms and Profiles. Burlington, VT, USA: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry, 2000.

  • 11

    Squires J, Bricker D & & Twombly E Ages & Stages Questionnaires®: Social-Emotional Second Edition (ASQ®:SE-2): A Parent-Completed Child Monitoring System for Social Emotional Behaviors. Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co., Inc., 2015.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 12

    Urbanus E, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, Cordeiro L, & van Rijn S. The behavioral profile of children aged 1–5 years with sex chromosome trisomy (47,XXX, 47,XXY, 47,XYY). American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part C, Seminars in Medical Genetics 2020 184 444455. (https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.c.31788)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 13

    Bayley N Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, 3 rd ed. San Antonio, TX, USA: Harcourt Assessment, 2006.

  • 14

    Wiig EH, Secord WA & & Semel E Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals—Preschool, 2 nd ed. (CELF Preschool-2). Toronto, Canada: The Psychological Corporation/A Harcourt Assessment Company, 2004.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 15

    Korkman M, Kirk U & & Kemp S The Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment II (NEPSY-II). San Antonio, TX, USA: Harcourt Assessment, 2007.

  • 16

    Dunn LM & & Dunn LM Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Manual, 3rd ed. USA: American Guidance Service, 1997.

  • 17

    Fenson L, Dale PS, Reznick JS, Thal D, Bates E, Hartung JP, Pethick S & & Reilly JS MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User’s Guide and Technical Manual. San Diego, CA, USA: Singular Publishing Group, Inc, 1993.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 18

    Urbanus E, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, Boada R, & van Rijn S. [Formula: See text] A cross-sectional study of early language abilities in children with sex chromosome trisomy (XXY, XXX, XYY) aged 1–6 years. Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence 2022 28 171196. (https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2021.1960959)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 19

    Urbanus E, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, Stumpel C, & van Rijn S. Structural and pragmatic language in young children with sex chromosome trisomy (XXX, XXY, XYY): predictive value for neurobehavioral problems one year later. Clinical Neuropsychologist 2023 14 126. (https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2022.2067078)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 20

    Simms MD. Language disorders in children: classification and clinical syndromes. Pediatric Clinics of North America 2007 54 437467, v. (https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pcl.2007.02.014)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 21

    Bouw N, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, Jansen AC, & van Rijn S. Early impact of X- and Y-chromosome variations (XXX, XXY, XYY) on social communication and social emotional development in 1–2-year-old children. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A 2022 188 19431953. (https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.a.62720)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 22

    Bouw N, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, & van Rijn S. The impact of sex chromosome trisomies (XXX, XXY, XYY) on early social cognition: social orienting, joint attention, and theory of mind. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 2022 37 6377. (https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acab042)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 23

    Bouw N, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, Cordeiro L, & van Rijn S. Early social behavior in young children with sex chromosome trisomies (XXX, XXY, XYY): profiles of observed social interactions and social impairments associated with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2022. (https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-022-05553-8)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 24

    Bouw N, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, Wilson RL, Van der Velde K, & van Rijn S. Early symptoms of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in 1–8 year old children with sex chromosome trisomies (XXX, XXY, XYY), and the predictive value of joint attention. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2022 [epub]. (https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-022-02070-y)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 25

    Bouw N, Swaab H, & van Rijn S. Early preventive intervention for young children with sex chromosome trisomies (XXX, XXY, XYY): supporting social cognitive development using a neurocognitive training program targeting facial emotion understanding. Frontiers in Psychiatry 2022 13 807793. (https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.807793)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 26

    Blair C, & Diamond A. Biological processes in prevention and intervention: the promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing school failure. Development and Psychopathology 2008 20 899911. (https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579408000436)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 27

    Kuiper KC, Swaab H, Tartaglia NR, Cordeiro L, & van Rijn S. Emotional reactivity and expressivity in young children with sex chromosome trisomies: evidence from psychophysiological and observational data. Child Neuropsychology: a Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence 2022 119. (https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2022.2102161)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 28

    Kuiper KC, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, & van Rijn S. (Not) getting what you want: emotion regulation strategies during frustrating task in young children with sex chromosome trisomies. Endocrine Connections [epub].

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 29

    Kuiper KC, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, van Buggenhout G, Wouters C, & van Rijn S. The developmental impact of sex chromosome trisomies on emerging executive functions in young children: evidence from neurocognitive tests and daily life skills. Genes, Brain, and Behavior 2022 21 e12811. (https://doi.org/10.1111/gbb.12811)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 30

    Kuiper K, Swaab H, Tartaglia N, & van Rijn S. Early developmental impact of sex chromosome trisomies on attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder symptomology in young children. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A 2021 185 36643674. (https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.a.62418)

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 31

    Rourke BP, Bakker DJ, Fisk JL & & Strang JD Child Neuropsychology. New York, NY, USA: Guilford Press, 1983.